Saltar para o conteúdo

Sono: diferenças entre revisões

Origem: Wikipédia, a enciclopédia livre.
Conteúdo apagado Conteúdo adicionado
CarsracBot (discussão | contribs)
m Bot: Adicionando: ms:Tidur
Linha 1: Linha 1:
{{Otheruses}}
{{ver desambiguação}}


[[Image:Sleepy men.JPG|thumb|300px|Sleepy men, Tehran, Iran]]
'''Sleep''' is a natural state of bodily rest observed throughout the [[animal kingdom]]. It is common to all [[mammals]] and [[birds]], and is also seen in many [[reptiles]], [[amphibians]] and [[fish]]. In humans, other mammals, and a substantial majority of other animals which have been studied — such as fish, birds, [[ants]], and [[Drosophilidae|fruit-flies]] — regular sleep is essential for survival.<ref> {{cite book |last= |first= |authorlink= |coauthors= |editor= |others= Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council |title= Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research |origdate= |origyear= |origmonth= |url= http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10732&page=121 |format= |accessdate= |accessyear= |accessmonth= |edition= |series= |volume= |date= |year= 2003 |month= |publisher= The National Academies Press |location= |isbn= 978-0-309-08903-6 |oclc= |doi= |id= |pages= pg 121 |chapter= |chapterurl= |quote= Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983).}}</ref> However, its purposes are only partly clear and are the subject of intense research.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://thesciencenetwork.org/programs/Waking%20Up%20To%20Sleep%202006/ |title= Waking Up To Sleep |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |author= |last= Bingham |first= Roger |authorlink= |coauthors= Terrence Sejnowski, Jerry Siegel, Mark Eric Dyken, Charles Czeisler, Paul Shaw, Ralph Greenspan, Satchin Panda, Philip Low, Robert Stickgold, Sara Mednick, Allan Pack, Luis de Lecea, David Dinges, Dan Kripke, Giulio Tononi |year= 2007 |month= February |format= Several conference videos |work= |publisher= The Science Network |pages= |language= |doi= |archiveurl= |archivedate= |quote= }}</ref>


== Physiology ==
'''Sono''' (do [[latim]] ''somnu'', com o mesmo significado) é um estado ordinário de [[consciência]], complementar ao da [[Vigília|vigília]] (ou estado desperto), em que há repouso normal e periódico, caracterizado, tanto no ser humano como nos outros animais superiores, pela suspensão temporária da atividade perceptivo-sensorial e motora voluntária.


In mammals and birds the measurement of eye movement during sleep is used to divide sleep into the two broad types of [[Rapid eye movement sleep|Rapid Eye Movement]] (REM) and [[Non-rapid eye movement sleep|Non-Rapid Eye Movement]] (NREM) sleep. Each type has a distinct set of associated physiological, neurological and psychological features.
Ao dizer-se '''complementar''', em conjugação com ordinário, quer-se significar tão somente que, na maioria dos indivíduos (com destaque, aqui, para os humanos), tais estados de '''consciência''' alternam-se, complementando-se ordinária, periódica e regularmente.


Sleep proceeds in cycles of REM and the four stages of NREM, the order normally being:
O Estado de '''sono''' é caracterizado por um padrão de ondas cerebrais típico, essencialmente diferente do padrão do estado de [[Vigília|vigília]], bem como do verificado nos demais estados de [[consciência]]. '''Dormir''', nesta acepção, significa passar do estado de '''[[Vigília|vigília]]''' para o estado de sono.
:''stages 1 > 2 > 3 > 4 > 3 > 2 > REM''.


In humans this cycle is on average 90 to 110 minutes,<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.sleepdisorderchannel.com/stages/ |title= Sleep Stages. Overview, Waking, Non-REM, REM, Sleep Cycle, Factors, Age |accessdate= 2008-02-10 |last= Swierzewski |first= Stanley J., M.D. |date= 01 December 2000, reviewed 04 December 2007 |format= |work= |publisher= Sleep Channel, Healthcommunities.com |quote= }}</ref> with a greater amount of stages 3 and 4 early in the night and more REM later in the night. Each phase may have a distinct physiological function. Drugs such as [[sleeping pill]]s and [[alcoholic beverage]]s can suppress certain stages of sleep (see ''[[Sleep deprivation]]''). This can result in a sleep that exhibits loss of consciousness but does not fulfill its physiological functions.
No ser humano, o [[Período|ciclo]] do sono é formado por cinco estágios e dura cerca de noventa minutos (podendo chegar a 120 minutos). Ele se repete durante quatro ou cinco vezes durante o sono.


[[Allan Rechtschaffen]] and Anthony Kales originally outlined the criteria for staging sleep in 1968. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) updated the staging rules in 2007.
Ao dormir, os [[sentidos]] perdem-se na seguinte ordem: [[visão]], [[paladar]], [[olfato]], [[audição]] e [[tato]]. O tato desperta ao mais leve toque sobre a pele. É o vigia do corpo adormecido.


[[Image:SWS.jpg|thumb|165px|right|Stage 4 Sleep. EEG highlighted by red box.]]
Ao despertar, os [[sentidos]] voltam nesta ordem: [[tato]], [[audição]], [[visão]], [[paladar]] e [[olfato]].
[[Image:REM.png|thumb|165px|right|REM Sleep. EEG highlighted by red box. Eye movements highlighted by red line.]]


===Stages of sleep===
Do que se tem registro na literatura especializada, o período mais longo que uma pessoa já conseguiu ficar sem dormir foi de onze dias. (Por curiosidade, [[Napoleão Bonaparte]] dormia quatro horas por noite; já [[Albert Einstein]] precisava de dez horas de sono).


Criteria for '''REM''' sleep include not only rapid eye movements but also a rapid low voltage [[EEG]]. In mammals, at least, low muscle tone is also seen. Most memorable dreaming occurs in this stage. '''NREM''' accounts for 75&ndash;80% of total sleep time in normal human adults. In NREM sleep, there is relatively little dreaming. Non-REM encompasses four stages; stages 1 and 2 are considered 'light sleep', and 3 and 4 'deep sleep' or [[slow-wave sleep]], SWS. They are differentiated solely using EEG, unlike REM sleep which is characterized by rapid eye movements and relative absence of muscle tone. In non-REM sleep there are often limb movements, and [[parasomnia]]s such as [[sleepwalking]] may occur. A [[cyclical alternating pattern]] may sometimes be observed during a stage.
Em geral, quanto mais velho um ser humano fica, de menos tempo diário de sono necessita<ref>[http://www.medicinageriatrica.com.br/2007/06/02/saude-geriatria/fisiologia-do-sono-do-idoso/ Fisiologia do sono do idoso]</ref>segue esquema:


NREM consists of four stages according to the 2007 AASM standards:
* De 0 anos a 3 anos: mais de 10 horas de sono;
* During '''Stage N1''' the brain transitions from [[alpha waves]] (having a frequency of 8 to 13 [[Hertz|Hz]], common to people who are awake) to [[theta rhythm|theta waves]] (frequency of 4 to 7 Hz). This stage is sometimes referred to as ''somnolence'', or "drowsy sleep". Associated with the onset of sleep during N1 may be sudden twitches and [[hypnic jerk]]s also known as positive [[myoclonus]]. Some people may also experience [[hypnagogic hallucination]]s during this stage, which can be more troublesome to the subject. During N1 the subject loses some [[muscle tone]] and conscious awareness of the external environment.
* De 4 anos a 6 anos: em media 10 horas de sono;
* '''Stage N2''', is characterized by "[[sleep spindle]]s" (12 to 16 Hz) and "[[K-complex]]es." During this stage, muscular activity as measured by [[electromyography]] (EMG) lowers and conscious awareness of the external environment disappears. This stage occupies 45 to 55% of total sleep.
* De 7 anos a 8 anos: em media 9 horas de sono;
* In '''Stage N3''', the [[delta wave]]s, also called ''delta rhythms'' (0.5 to 4 Hz) make up less than 50% of the total wave-patterns. This is considered part of deep or slow-wave sleep (SWS) and appears to function primarily as a transition into stage N4. This is the stage in which [[night terrors]], [[bedwetting]], [[sleepwalking]] and [[somniloquy|sleep-talking]] occur.
* De 9 anos a 14 anos: em media 8 horas de sono;
* In '''Stage N4''', delta-waves make up ''more'' than 50% of the wave-patterns. Stages N3 and N4 are the deepest forms of sleep; N4 is effectively a deeper version of N3, in which the deep-sleep characteristics, such as delta-waves, are more pronounced. As of new AASM guidelines, the determination between stage 3 and stage 4 sleep is inconsequential and both may be considered delta sleep or slow wave sleep. Therefore, in a recent ruling by the AASM, in order to make precision the scoring guidelines, stage four had been disbanded, and left is the stage of sleep N3 to describe the delta sleep attributed to it.<ref name="Pinel, J.P.J.">{{cite web |url= http://web.mst.edu/~psyworld/general/sleepstages/sleepstages.pdf |title= Stages of Sleep |accessdate= 2008-06-15 |author= Psychology World |last= |first= |coauthors= |year= 1998 |format= PDF |work= |publisher= |archiveurl= |archivedate= |quote= }}</ref>
* De 15 anos a 16 anos: em media 7 horas de sono;
* De 17 anos a 50 anos: em media 6 horas de sono;
* De 51 anos a x anos: Podem dormir menos de 6 horas, havendo variações.


Both REM sleep and NREM sleep stages 3 and 4 are homeostatically driven; that is, if a person or animal is selectively deprived of one of these, it rebounds once uninhibited sleep again is allowed. This suggests that both are essential to the functions of the sleep process.
Entretanto, podem haver variações nesse quadro, pois o sono é muito subjetivo, enquanto algumas precisam por volta de 4 horas de sono, outras precisam de 9 a 10 horas.


==Introdução==
===Sleep timing===
[[Imagem:A child sleeping.jpg|thumb|250px|Uma criança adormecida]]


Sleep timing is controlled by the [[Circadian rhythm|circadian clock]], by [[homeostasis]] and, in humans, by willed behavior. The circadian clock, an inner time-keeping, temperature-fluctuating, enzyme-controlling device, works in tandem with [[adenosine]], a neurotransmitter which inhibits many of the bodily processes that are associated with wakefulness. Adenosine is created over the course of the day; high levels of adenosine lead to sleepiness. In diurnal animals, sleepiness occurs as the circadian element causes the release of the hormone [[melatonin]] and a gradual decrease in core body temperature. The timing is affected by one's [[chronotype]]. It is the circadian rhythm which determines the ideal timing of a correctly structured and restorative sleep episode.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Wyatt |first=James K. |coauthors=Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan |year=1999 |month=October |title= Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day |journal=Am J Physiol |volume=277 |issue=4 |pages=R1152–R1163. Fulltext |id= |url= http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/277/4/R1152 | accessdate=2007-11-25 |quote= |pmid= 10516257 }}</ref>
Pode definir-se '''sono''' como "um período de repouso para o corpo e a mente, durante o qual a volição e a consciência estão em inactividade parcial ou completa" (DORLAND citado por ROPER; LOGAN; TIERNEY, 1993, 466). Já FRIEDMAN (1995, 1827), define sono como "sendo o desencadear deliberado de uma alteração ou redução do estado consciente, que dura, em média 8 horas (...) tendo início sensivelmente à mesma hora, em cada período de 24 horas, e (...) resultando, geralmente, em sensação de energia física, psíquica e intelectual restabelecida".


Homeostatic sleep propensity, the need for sleep as a function of the amount of time elapsed since the last adequate sleep episode, is also important and must be balanced against the circadian element for satisfactory sleep. Along with corresponding messages from the circadian clock, this tells the body it needs to sleep.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.helpguide.org/life/sleeping.htm |title= Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |author= |last= de Benedictis |first= Tina, Ph.D. |coauthors= Heather Larson, Gina Kemp, M.A., Suzanne Barston, Robert Segal, M.A. |year= 2007 |publisher= Helpguide.org |quote= }}</ref> Sleep offset, awakening, is primarily determined by circadian rhythm. A normal person who regularly awakens at an early hour will generally not be able to sleep much later than the person's normal waking time, even if moderately sleep deprived.
Existem, pois, várias definições do sono apresentadas por diferentes autores, e, no geral, complementam-se umas às outras.


=== Optimal amount in humans ===
De acordo com FONTAINE, BRIGGS e POPE-SMITH (2001), o sono é importante para a recuperação da [[saúde]] em situação de [[doença]], enquanto a privação deste pode afetar a regeneração celular assim como a total recuperação da função imunitária.
==== Adults ====
The optimal amount of sleep is not a meaningful concept unless the timing of that sleep is seen in relation to an individual's [[circadian rhythm]]s. A person's major sleep episode is relatively inefficient and inadequate when it occurs at the "wrong" time of day. The timing is correct when the following two circadian markers occur after the middle of the sleep episode but before awakening:<ref>{{cite journal |last=Wyatt |first=James K. |coauthors=Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan |year=1999 |month=October |title= Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day |journal=Am J Physiol |volume=277 |issue=4 |pages=R1152–R1163 |id= |url= http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/277/4/R1152
|accessdate=2007-11-25 |quote=... significant homeostatic and circadian modulation of sleep structure, with the highest sleep efficiency occurring in sleep episodes bracketing the melatonin maximum and core body temperature minimum |pmid= 10516257 }}</ref>
* maximum concentration of the hormone melatonin, and
* minimum core body temperature.


The [[National Sleep Foundation|National Sleep Foundation in the United States]] maintains that eight to nine hours of sleep for adult humans is optimal and that sufficient sleep benefits alertness, memory and problem solving, and overall health, as well as reducing the risk of accidents.<ref name="let sleep work for you">{{cite web | title = "Let Sleep Work for You" provided by the National Sleep Foundation | url = http://www.sleepfoundation.org/site/c.huIXKjM0IxF/b.2421185/k.7198/Let_Sleep_Work_for_You.htm}}</ref> A widely publicized 2003 study<ref>Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003 Mar 15;26(2):117–26.</ref> performed at the [[University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine]] demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.
O sono divide-se em dois estágios fisiologicamente distintos:


However, a [[University of California, San Diego]] psychiatry study of more than one million adults found that people who live the longest self-report sleeping for six to seven hours each night.<ref>{{cite web | author = Rhonda Rowland | title = Experts challenge study linking sleep, life span |date=2002-02-15 | url = http://archives.cnn.com/2002/HEALTH/02/14/sleep.study/index.html | accessdate = 2007-04-22 }}</ref> Another study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women showed similar results.<ref>Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR et al (2004) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=15164896 A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women] ''Sleep'' 1;27(3):440-4</ref> Other studies show that "sleeping more than 7 to 8 hours per day has been consistently associated with increased mortality", though this study suggests the cause is probably other factors such as depression and socio-economic status which would correlate statistically. <ref>Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ et al (2006) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=pubmed&cmd=Retrieve&dopt=AbstractPlus&list_uids=16895254 Correlates of long sleep duration] ''Sleep'' 29(7):881-889</ref> It has been suggested that the correlation between lower sleep hours and reduced morbidity only occurs with those who wake after less sleep naturally, rather than those who use an alarm.
*''[[REM]]'' (''Rapid Eye Movement'' ou "Movimento Rápido dos Olhos"); e


[[Image:Sleeping Kutchi.jpg|thumb|left|A Koli Wada woman sleeping in Nirona village]]
*''[[NREM]]'' (''Non Rapid Eye Movement'' ou "Movimento Não Rápido dos Olhos").
Causal links are currently speculative: the available data may only reflect comorbid depression, socioeconomic status, or even alcohol use, for example.<ref>Irwin MR, Ziegler M (2005) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=15642774 Sleep deprivation potentiates activation of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses in abstinent alcoholics] ''Hypertension'' 45(2):252-7</ref> These studies cannot be used to determine optimal sleep habits, only correlation — and [[correlation does not imply causation|empirically observed correlation is a necessary but not sufficient condition for causality]]. A need for nine or ten hours of sleep a day, or only five to six, may or may not have the same cause as the shortened life span. In other words, long or short sleep duration itself has not been shown to be a cause of early death.


Researchers from the [[University of Warwick]] and [[University College London]] have found that lack of sleep can more than double the risk of death from [[cardiovascular disease]], but that too much sleep can also double the risk of death.<ref>{{cite web | title = "Researchers say lack of sleep doubles risk of death… but so can too much sleep" | url = http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/newsandevents/pressreleases/researchers_say_lack/}}</ref><ref>Jane E. Ferrie, Martin J. Shipley, Francesco P. Cappuccio, Eric Brunner, Michelle A. Miller, Meena Kumari, and Michael G. Marmot. “A prospective study of change in sleep duration; associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort”. SLEEP Journal.</ref> Professor Francesco Cappuccio said: “Short sleep has been shown to be a risk factor for weight gain, [[hypertension]] and Type 2 diabetes sometimes leading to mortality but in contrast to the short sleep-mortality association it appears that no potential mechanisms by which long sleep could be associated with increased mortality have yet been investigated. Some candidate causes for this include depression, low socioeconomic status and cancer-related fatigue. [...] In terms of prevention, our findings indicate that consistently sleeping around 7 hours per night is optimal for health and a sustained reduction may predispose to ill-health.”
==O Sono REM==
O sono [[REM]], caracteriza-se por uma intensa atividade registrada no [[Eletroencefalograma]] ([[Eletroencefalograma|EEG]]) seguida por flacidez, paralisia funcional, dos músculos esqueléticos. Nesta fase, a atividade cerebral é semelhante à do estado de [[vigília]]. Deste modo, o [[sono]] [[REM]] é também denominado por vários autores como [[sono]] [[Paradoxo|paradoxal]], podendo mesmo falar-se em estado [[Dissociação|dissociativo]].


====Hours by age====
Nesta fase do sono, a atividade onírica é intensa, sendo sobretudo [[sonho]]s envolvendo situações emocionalmente muito fortes.
[[Image:A Child Sleeping.jpg|thumb|A child sleeping]]


Children need a greater amount of sleep per day than adults to develop and function properly: up to 18 hours for [[newborn]] babies, with a declining rate as a child ages.<ref name="let sleep work for you" /><ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.helpguide.org/life/sleeping.htm |title= Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |author= |last= de Benedictis |first= Tina, Ph.D. |coauthors= Heather Larson, Gina Kemp, M.A., Suzanne Barston, Robert Segal, M.A. |year= 2007 |publisher= Helpguide.org |quote= }}</ref> A newborn baby spends almost half of its sleep time in REM-sleep. By the age of five or so, only a bit over two hours are spent in REM.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.npi.ucla.edu/sleepresearch/encarta/Article.htm | title= Sleep |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last= Siegel |first= Jerome M. |year= 1999 |format= |work= Encarta Encyclopedia |publisher= Microsoft |quote= }}</ref>
É durante essa fase que é feita integração da atividade cotidiana, isto é, a separação do comum do importante. Este representa 20 a 25% do tempo total de sono e surge em intervalos de sessenta a noventa minutos. É essencial para o bem-estar físico e psicológico do indivíduo.


{| class="wikitable"
==O Sono NREM==
|-
O sono [[NREM]] (ou não-REM) ocupa cerca de 75% do tempo do sono e divide-se em quatro períodos distintos conhecidos como ''estágios 1, 2, 3 e 4''.
! Age
! Average amount of sleep per day
|-
| Newborn
| up to 18 hours
|-
| 1-12 months
| 14–18 hours
|-
| 1-3 years
| 12-15 hours
|-
| 3-5 years
| 11-13 hours
|-
| 5-12 years
| 9-11 hours
|-
| Adolescents
| 9-10 hours
|-
| Adults, including elderly
| 7-8 (+) hours
|-
| Pregnant women
| 8 (+) hours
|}


===Sleep debt===
*'''Estágio 1:'''
Começa com uma ''sonolência''. Dura aproximadamente cinco minutos. A pessoa adormece. É caracterizado por um '''[[Eletroencefalograma|EEG]]''' semelhante ao do '''estado de [[vigília]]'''. Esse estágio tem uma duração de um a dois minutos, estando o indivíduo facilmente despertável. Predominam sensações de vagueio, pensamentos incertos, '''[[Neurologia|mioclonias]]''' das mãos e dos pés, lenta contração e '''[[Oftalmologia|dilatação pupilar]]'''. Nessa fase, a '''[[Onirismo|atividade onírica]]''' está sempre relacionada com acontecimentos vividos recentemente.


{{main|Sleep debt}}
*'''Estágio 2:'''
Sleep debt is the effect of not getting quite enough rest and sleep; a large debt causes mental, emotional and physical fatigue. Scientists do not agree on how much sleep debt it is possible to accumulate, nor on whether the prevalence of sleep debt among adults has changed appreciably in the industrialized world in recent decades. It is likely that children are sleeping less than previously in Western societies.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Iglowstein | first = Ivo | coauthors = Oskar G. Jenni, MD, Luciano Molinari, PhD and Remo H. Largo, MD | year = 2003 | month = February | title = Sleep Duration From Infancy to Adolescence: Reference Values and Generational Trends | journal = Pediatrics | volume = 111 | issue = 2 | pages = pp. 302–307 | publisher = | doi = 10.1542/peds.111.2.302 | quote = Thus, the shift in the evening bedtime across cohorts accounted for the substantial decrease in sleep duration in younger children between the 1970s and the 1990s. ... [A] more liberal parental attitude toward evening bedtime in the past decades is most likely responsible for the bedtime shift and for the decline of sleep duration... | pmid = 12563055 }}</ref>
Caracteriza-se por a pessoa já dormir, porém não profundamente. Dura cerca de cinco a quinze minutos. O '''[[Eletroencefalograma|EEG]]''' mostra frequências de ondas mais lentas, aparecendo o '''complexo K'''. Nessa fase, os despertares por estimulação táctil, fala ou movimentos corporais são mais difíceis do que no anterior estágio. Aqui a '''[[Onirismo|atividade onírica]]''' já pode surgir sob a forma de '''[[sonho]]''' com uma história integrada.


== Functions ==
*'''Estágio 3:'''
The multiple theories proposed to explain the function of sleep are reflective of the as yet incomplete understanding of the subject.
Tem muitas semelhanças com o estágio 4, daí serem quase sempre associados em termos bibliográficos quando são caracterizados. Nessas fases, os estímulos necessários para acordar são maiores. Do estágio 3 para o estágio 4, há uma progressão da dificuldade de despertar. Esse estágio tem a duração de cerca de quinze a vinte minutos.


It is likely that sleep evolved to fulfill some primeval function, but has taken over multiple functions over time as organisms have evolved. An analogy would be that of the larynx, which performs multiple functions such as controlling the passage of food and air, phonation for communicating and social purposes, etc. These are all functions of the larynx but just one of them likely represents the original function. Some of the many proposed functions of sleep are as follows:
*'''Estágio 4:'''
São quarenta minutos de sono profundo. É muito difícil acordar alguém nessa fase de sono. Depois, '''a pessoa ''retorna'' ao terceiro estágio''' (por cinco minutos) e ao '''segundo estágio''' (por mais quinze minutos). Entra, então, no '''[[sono]] [[REM]]'''.


===Restoration===
Este estágio '''[[NREM]]''' do '''[[sono]]''' caracteriza-se pela secreção do '''[[hormônio]]''' do crescimento em grandes quantidades, promovendo a '''[[síntese protéica]]''', o crescimento e '''reparação tecidular''', inibindo, assim, o '''[[Metabolismo|catabolismo]]''' (cf. BOLANDER, 1998). O '''[[sono]] [[NREM]]''' tem, pois, um papel '''[[Metabolismo|anabólico]]''', sendo essencialmente um período de conservação e recuperação de '''[[energia]] física'''.


[[Wound healing]] has been shown to be affected by sleep. A study conducted by Gumustekin et al.<ref>Gumustekin, K., Seven, B., Karabulut, N., Aktas, O., Gursan, N., Aslan, S., Keles, M., Varoglu, E., & Dane S. (2004). Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats [Electronic version]. ''Neuroscience, 114,'' 1433-1442.</ref> in 2004 shows sleep deprivation hindering the [[healing]] of burns on rats.
==O ciclo do sono==
Um '''[[Período|ciclo]] do [[sono]]''' dura cerca de noventa minutos, ocorrendo quatro a cinco ciclos num período de sono noturno. Segundo LAVIE (1998, 45), o número de ciclos por noite depende do tempo do sono, acrescentando, ainda, que "o sono de uma pessoa jovem é, habitualmente, composto por quatro ou cinco desses ciclos, com tendência à redução com o avançar da idade". No entanto, o padrão comum varia entre quatro a cinco ciclos.
Durante o sono, o indivíduo passa, geralmente por ciclos repetitivos, começando pelo '''estágio 1''' do '''[[sono]] [[NREM]]''', ''progredindo até o'' '''estágio 4''', ''regride para o'' '''estágio 2''', e entra em '''[[sono]] [[REM]]'''. ''Volta de novo ao'' '''estágio 2''' e assim se repete novamente todo o ciclo ('''Figura 1''').
<!-- Figura I – Natureza cíclica do sono -->


It has also been shown that sleep deprivation affects the [[immune system]] and [[metabolism]]. In a study by Zager ''et al'' in 2007,<ref>Zager, A., Andersen, M. L., Ruiz, F. S., Antunes, I. B., & Tufik, S. (2007). Effects of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats [Electronic version]. ''Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 293,'' R504-R509.</ref> rats were deprived of sleep for 24 hours. When compared with a control group, the sleep-deprived rats' blood tests indicated a 20% decrease in [[white blood cell]] count, a significant change in the immune system.
Nos primeiros ciclos do [[sono]], os períodos de [[NREM]] (mais concretamente o estágio 3 e 4) têem uma duração maior que o [[REM]]. À medida que o [[sono]] vai progredindo, os estágios 3 e 4 começam a encurtar e o período [[REM]] começa a aumentar. Na primeira parte do [[sono]] predomina o [[NREM]], sendo os períodos [[REM]] mais duradouros na segunda metade.


A study by Bonnet and Arand<ref>Bonnet, M. H. & Arand, D. L. (2003). Insomnia, metabolic rate and sleep restoration [Electronic version]. ''Journal of Internal Medicine, 254,'' 23-31.</ref> in 2003 indicates that sleep affects metabolism. Comparing normal human sleepers and sleepers with ''sleep state misperception insomnia'', where patients complain of poor sleep but have normal sleep by electroencephalographic (EEG) criteria, the researchers found significantly greater metabolism values for the normal sleepers.
==Fatores Ambientais que Interferem no Sono==
Segundo ''Phipps'' (1995) o sono é uma das muitas ocorrências biológicas que tem lugar à mesma hora, cada 24 horas.
Este é marcado por uma intensa actividade cerebral e pela ocorrência de determinadas funções que são muito importantes para o organismo.


It has yet to be clearly proven that sleep ''duration'' affects [[somatic]] growth. One study by Jenni ''et al''<ref>Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., Caflisch, J. A., & Largo, R. H. (2007). Sleep duration from ages 1 to 10 years: Variability and stability in comparison with growth [Electronic version]. ''Pediatrics, 120,'' e769-e776.</ref> in 2007 recorded growth, height and weight, as correlated to parent-reported time-in-bed in 305 children over a period of nine years (age 1-10). It was found that "the variation of sleep duration among children does not seem to have an effect on growth". It has been shown that sleep, more specifically slow-wave sleep (SWS), does affect [[growth hormone]] levels in adult men. During eight hours sleep, Van Cauter, Leproult, and Plat<ref>Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., & Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow-wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men [Electronic version]. ''Journal of the American Medical Association, 284,'' 861-868.</ref> found that the men with a high percentage of SWS (average 24%) also had high growth hormone secretion, while subjects with a low percentage of SWS (average 9%) had low growth hormone secretion.
A necessidade diária de sono varia, não só de indivíduo para indivíduo (variação inter-individual), como também no mesmo indivíduo (variação intra-individual) de dia para dia.
Existem vários factores que contribuem para a alteração do padrão de sono, nomeadamente factores físicos, sócio-culturais, psicológicos, ambientais, etc. Segundo um estudo efectuado por DLIN e colaboradores (citados por THELAN, 1996), dentro do ambiente temos então:


There are multiple arguments supporting the restorative function of sleep. We feel rested after sleeping, and it is natural to assume that this is a basic purpose of sleep. Overall metabolic rate goes down during sleep and certain anabolic hormones such as growth hormones as mentioned above are secreted preferentially during sleep. Sleep among species is, in general, inversely related to the animal size and [[basal metabolic rate]]. Rats with a very high basal metabolic rate sleep for up to 14 hours a day whereas elephants and giraffes with lower BMRs sleep only 3-4 hours per day.
*'''Ruído:'''
O ruído pode ser visto como um perigo ambiental que cria desconforto e pode interferir com o sono e repouso do doente, uma vez que activa o [[sistema nervoso simpático]] cuja estimulação é responsável pelo estado de vigília ou alerta do indivíduo.


Energy conservation could as well have been accomplished by resting quiescent without shutting off the organism from the environment, potentially a dangerous situation. A sedentary non-sleeping animal is more likely to survive predators, while still preserving energy. Sleep therefore does something else other than conserving energy. Most interestingly, [[hibernation|hibernating]] animals that wake up from hibernation go into rebound sleep because of lack of sleep during the hibernation period. They are definitely well rested and are conserving energy during hibernation, but need sleep for something else.<ref name="pmid1945046">{{cite journal |author=Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM |title=Warming up for sleep? Ground squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation |journal=Neurosci. Lett. |volume=128 |issue=2 | pages = 581 |year=1991 |pmid=1945046 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->|url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0304-3940(91)90276-Y}}</ref> Rats kept awake indefinitely start dying after a few days.<ref> {{cite book |last= |first= |authorlink= |coauthors= |editor= |others= Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council |title= Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research |origdate= |origyear= |origmonth= |url= http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10732&page=121 |format= |accessdate= |accessyear= |accessmonth= |edition= |series= |volume= |date= |year= 2003 |month= |publisher= The National Academies Press |location= |isbn= 978-0-309-08903-6 |oclc= |doi= |id= |pages= pg 121 |chapter= |chapterurl= |quote= Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983).}} </ref>
*'''Luz:'''
Muitas são as pessoas que apresentam um nível de sensibilidade elevado à luz, sendo por isso facilmente perturbadas durante o sono mesmo que seja uma luz de pouca intensidade.


===Anabolic/catabolic ===
*'''Temperatura:'''
[[Non-REM]] sleep may be an [[Anabolism|anabolic]] state marked by physiological processes of growth and rejuvenation of the organism's immune, nervous, muscular, and skeletal systems (but see above). Wakefulness may perhaps be viewed as a cyclical, temporary, hyperactive [[catabolic]] state during which the organism acquires nourishment and procreates.
É importante que possa existir um maior controle sobre a [[temperatura]] do ambiente externo, e que este controle seja feito de forma cuidadosa. Tendo em conta que a [[temperatura corporal]] atinge o seu pico ao final da tarde ou princípio da noite e depois vai baixando progressivamente atingindo o ponto mais baixo ao início da manhã, uma diminuição ou um aumento da temperatura ambiente faz, geralmente, acordar a pessoa ou cria-lhe um certo desconforto que o impossibilita de dormir.


===Ontogenesis===
Também podem ocorrer "sensações" de fuga ou luta durante os últimos estágios do sono NREM, como ouvir o som do despertador tocando, o choro do bebê ou um chamado pelo seu nome.
According to the [[ontogeny|ontogenetic]] hypothesis of REM sleep, the activity occurring during neonatal REM sleep (or active sleep) seems to be particularly important to the developing organism (Marks et al., 1995). Studies investigating the effects of deprivation of active sleep have shown that deprivation early in life can result in behavioral problems, permanent sleep disruption, decreased brain mass (Mirmiran et al. 1983), and an abnormal amount of neuronal cell death (Morrissey, Duntley & Anch, 2004).


REM sleep appears to be important for development of the brain. REM sleep occupies majority of time of sleep of infants, which spend most of their time sleeping. Among different species, the more immature the baby is born, the more time it spends in REM sleep. Proponents also suggest that REM-induced muscle inhibition in the presence of brain activation exists to allow for brain development by activating the synapses yet without any motor consequences which may get the infant in trouble. Additionally, REM deprivation results in developmental abnormalities later in life.
Fonte: Corpo e Saúde, Seleções do Reader's Digest


However, this does not explain why older adults still need REM sleep, and why the fraction of time spent does not change significantly {{Fact|date=May 2008}} as one ages. Aquatic mammal infants do not have REM sleep in infancy. REM sleep in those animals increases as they age. Obviously, REM sleep is not needed for development in these animals.
==Referências mitológicas==
Segundo [[mitologia grega|mitologias gregas]], o deus [[Hipnos]] seria a própria personificação do sono, e [[Oniro]], a do sonho.


===Memory processing===
==Dicas de como dormir melhor==
Scientists have shown numerous ways in which sleep is related to [[memory]]. In a study conducted by Turner, Drummond, Salamat, and Brown<ref>Turner, T. H., Drummond, S. P. A., Salamat, J. S., & Brown, G. G. (2007). Effects of 42 hr sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory [Electronic version]. ''Neuropsychology, 21,'' 787-795.</ref> [[working memory]] was shown to be affected by sleep deprivation. Working memory is important because it keeps information active for further processing and supports higher-level [[cognitive functions]] such as [[decision making]], [[reasoning]], and [[episodic memory]]. Turner ''et al.'' allowed 18 women and 22 men to sleep only 26 minutes per night over a 4-day period. Subjects were given initial [[cognitive tests]] while well rested and then tested again twice a day during the 4 days of sleep deprivation. On the final test the average working memory span of the sleep deprived group had dropped by 38% in comparison to the control group.
*Procure dormir e despertar sempre nos mesmos horários para facilitar o seu relógio interno ([[relógio biológico]]).
*Evite [[álcool|bebidas alcóolicas]].
*Bebidas que contenham [[cafeína]], tais como [[café]], e teína, tais como [[chá preto]] e [[chá mate]], devem ser evitados antes de dormir, bem como [[refrigerante]]s (especialmente as cafeinadas, tais como refrigerantes de [[Cola acuminata|cola]]).
*Não fume.
*O quarto de dormir deve ser silencioso, escuro e com temperatura amena.
*Desligue a [[televisão|TV]] antes de se deitar e, de preferência, assista a televisão meia hora ou uma hora antes de ir para a cama.
*A alimentação antes de dormir deve ser leve e de fácil digestão.
*A [[atividade física]] facilita o sono se for feita regularmente pelo menos 4 horas antes de dormir.
*Não tome remédios sem orientação médica.


Memory also seems to be affected differently by certain stages of sleep such as REM and [[slow-wave sleep]] (SWS). In one study cited in Born, Rasch, and Gais<ref>Born, J., Rasch, J., & Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember [Electronic version]. ''Neuroscientist, 12,'' 410.</ref> multiple groups of human subjects were used: wake control groups and sleep test groups. Sleep and wake groups were taught a task and then tested on it both on early and late nights, with the order of nights balanced across participants. When the subjects' brains were scanned during sleep, hypnograms revealed that SWS was the dominant sleep stage during the early night representing around 23% on average for sleep stage activity. The early night test group performed 16% better on the [[declarative memory]] test than the control group. During late night sleep, REM became the most active sleep stage at about 24%, and the late night test group performed 25% better on the [[procedural memory]] test than the control group. This indicates that procedural memory benefits from late REM-rich sleep whereas declarative memory benefits from early SWS-rich sleep.
==O dia do sono==
No dia 23 de março, comemora-se o dia do sono.


Another study conducted by Datta<ref>Datta, S. (2000). Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity [Electronic version]. ''The Journal of Neuroscience, 20,'' 8607-8613.</ref> indirectly supports these results. The subjects chosen were 22 male rats. A box was constructed where a single rat could move freely from one end to the other. The bottom of the box was made of a steel grate. A light would shine in the box accompanied by a sound. After a 5 second delay an electrical shock would be applied. Once the shock commenced the rat could move to the other end of the box, ending the shock immediately. The rat could also use the 5-second delay to move to the other end of the box and avoid the shock entirely. The length of the shock never exceeded 5 seconds. This was repeated 30 times for half the rats. The other half, the control group, was placed in the same trial but the rats were shocked regardless of their reaction. After each of the training sessions the rat would be placed in a recording cage for 6 hours of polygraphic recordings. This process was repeated for 3 consecutive days. This study found that during the post-trial sleep recording session rats spent 25.47% more time in REM sleep after learning trials than after control trials. These trials support the results of the Born et al. study, indicating an obvious correlation between REM sleep and [[procedural knowledge]].
==Referências bibliográficas==
*BOLANDER, Verolyn R., Necessidades Humanas, in BOLANDER, Verolyn R – Sorensen e Luckmann- Enfermagem fundamental: Abordagem Psicofisiológica, 1ª Edição, Lisboa: Lusodidacta, 1998, ISBN: 972-96610-6-5, pp. 307- 327.
*CRUZ, Arménio Guardado; SILVA, Carlos Fernandes – Consequência do Trabalho por Turnos, Revista Sinais Vitais, Coimbra, ISSN: 0872-8844, N.º 3, 1995 (Maio), pp. 37-42.
*LAVIE, Peretz – O Mundo Encantado do Sono, Lisboa: Climepsi Editores, 1998, ISBN: 972-8449-10-0, 255 p.
*PHIPPS, W. J.; et al. – Enfermagem Médico-Cirúrgica- Conceitos e Prática Clínica. 2ª Edição em Português, Lisboa: Lusodidacta, 1998, ISBN: 972-96610-6-5, pp. 2389-2391.
*REIMÃO R, ed. Sono estudo abrangente. 2 ed. São Paulo, Editora Atheneu, 1996.
*REIMÃO R. Durma Bem. São Paulo, Editora Atheneu, 1998.
*REIMÃO R, ed. Sono, sonho e seus distúrbios. São Paulo, Frôntis Editorial, 1999.
*REIMÃO R, ed. Sleep: Latinamerican References 1895-1995. São Paulo, Frôntis Editorial, 2000. 136p.
*REIMÃO R, ed. Avanços em Medicina do Sono. São Paulo, Associação Paulista de Medicina, Zeppelini Editorial, 2001.
*REIMÃO R, ed. Sono: Atualidades. São Paulo, Associação Paulista de Medicina, 2006. 238 p.
*VEIGA, J.M. - Cuidados de Enfermagem no Sono. Revista Sinais Vitais, Coimbra, ISSN: 0872-8844, N.o 3, 1995 (Maio), pp. 33-36.
*VALLE, L.E, REIMÃO R, ROSSINI S, ed. Segredos do sono. São Paulo, Tecmedd, 2008.
Corpo e Saúde, Seleções do Reader's Digest, pg 96, 97


Another interesting observation of the Datta study is that the learning group spent 180% more time in SWS than did the control group during the post-trial sleep-recording session. This phenomenon is supported by a study performed by Kudrimoti, Barnes, and McNaughton.<ref>Kudrimoti, H. S., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: Effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics [Electronic version]. ''The Journal of Neuroscience, 19,'' 4090-4101.</ref> This study shows that after spatial exploration activity, patterns of [[Hippocampus|hippocampal]] place cells are reactivated during SWS following the experiment. In a study by Kudrimoti ''et al.'' seven rats were run through a linear track using rewards on either end. The rats would then be placed in the track for 30 minutes to allow them to adjust (PRE), then they ran the track with reward based training for 30 minutes (RUN), and then they were allowed to rest for 30 minutes. During each of these three periods [[Electroencephalograph|EEG]] data were collected for information on the rats’ sleep stages. Kudrimoti ''et al.'' computed the mean firing rates of hippocampal place cells during pre-behavior SWS (PRE) and three 10-minute intervals in post-behavior SWS (POST) by averaging across 22 track-running sessions from seven rats. The results showed that 10 minutes after the trial RUN session there was a 12% increase in the mean firing rate of hippocampal place cells from the PRE level, however after 20 minutes the mean firing rate returned rapidly toward the PRE level. The elevated firing of hippocampal place cells during SWS after spatial exploration could explain why there were elevated levels of SWS sleep in Datta’s study as it also dealt with a form of spatial exploration.
=={{Ver também}}==
{{wikiquote|Sono}}
* [[Sono polifásico]]
* [[Apnéia do sono]]
* [[Bruxismo]]
* [[Consciência]]
* [[Enurese noturna]]
* [[Insônia]]
* [[Narcolepsia]]
* [[NREM]]
* [[REM]]
* [[Ronco]]
* [[Sonho]]
* [[Sonambulismo]]
* [[Vigília]]
* [[Adormecer]]
* [[Privação do sono]]
* [[Sonífero]]
* [[Neurofisiologia]]


The different studies all suggest that there is a correlation between sleep and the many complex functions of memory.
== Referências ==


{{further|[[Sleep and learning]], [[Sleep and creativity]]}}
<references />


===Preservation===
=={{Ligações externas}}==
The "Preservation and Protection" theory holds that sleep serves an adaptive function. It protects the person during that portion of the 24-hour day in which being awake, and hence roaming around, would place the individual at greatest risk. Organisms do not require 24 hours to feed themselves and meet other necessities. From this perspective of adaptation, organisms are safer by staying out of harm's way where potentially they could be prey to other, stronger organisms. They sleep at times that maximize their safety, given their physical capacities and their habitats. (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Webb, 1982).


However, this theory fails to explain why the brain disengages from the external environment during normal sleep. Another argument against the theory is that sleep is not simply a passive consequence of removing the animal from the environment, but is a "drive": animals alter their behaviors in order to obtain sleep. Therefore, circadian regulation is more than sufficient to explain periods of activity and quiescence that are adaptive to an organism, but the more peculiar specializations of sleep probably serve different and unknown functions.
* [http://www.cienciaviva.pt/healthxxi/topics.asp?lang=pt&accao=cnl2_doc_pt Perturbações do sono]
* [http://www.jornalcomunicacao.ufpr.br/node/1865 De olhos bem fechados - Noites bem dormidas podem valer dias melhores]


Moreover, the preservation theory does not explain why carnivores like lions, which are on top of the food chain, sleep the most. By the preservation logic, these top carnivores should not need any sleep at all. Preservation does not explain why aquatic mammals sleep while moving. Lethargy during these vulnerable hours would do the same, and will be more advantageous because the animal will be quiescent but still be able to respond to environmental challenges like predators etc. Sleep rebound that occurs after a sleepless night will be maladaptive, but still occurs for a reason. For example, a zebra falling asleep the day after it spent the sleeping time running from a lion is more and not less vulnerable to predation.


== Dreaming ==
[[Categoria:Distúrbios do sono]]
{{main|Dream}}
[[Categoria:Medicina do Sono]]
Dreaming is the perception of sensory images during sleep, in a sequence which the sleeper/dreamer usually perceives more as an apparent participant than an observer. Dreaming is stimulated by the [[pons]] and mostly occurs during the [[REM phase of sleep]].
[[Categoria:Neurologia]]

[[Categoria:Neurologia infantil]]
People have proposed many [[hypothesis|hypotheses]] about the functions of dreaming. [[Sigmund Freud]] postulated that dreams are the symbolic expression of frustrated desires that had been relegated to the [[subconscious]], and he used [[dream interpretation]] in the form of [[psychoanalysis]] to uncover these desires. Scientists have become skeptical about the Freudian interpretation, and place more emphasis on dreaming as a requirement for organization and consolidation of recent [[memory]] and experience. See Freud:[[The Interpretation of Dreams]]
[[Categoria:Sono]]

[[Rosalind Cartwright]] stated that

{{quotation|One Function of dreams may be to restore our sense of competence.... it is also probable that in many times of stress, dreams have more work to do in resolving our problems and are thus more salient and memorable.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.sundayobserver.lk/2008/06/22/spe18.asp|title=Dream your dreams away|last=Nanayakkara|first=Anushka|work=[[The Sunday Observer]]|accessdate=2008-07-09}}</ref>|Rosalind Cartwright|[[The Sunday Observer]]}}

[[Allan Hobson|J. Allan Hobson's]] and [[Robert McCarley|Robert McCarley's]] [[activation synthesis theory]] proposes that dreams are caused by the [[random]] firing of [[neurons]] in the [[cerebral cortex]] during the REM period. According to the theory, the [[forebrain]] then creates a [[story]] in an attempt to reconcile and make sense of the nonsensical sensory information presented to it, hence the odd nature of many dreams.<ref>Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348.</ref>

== Effect of food and drink on sleep ==
===Drowsiness===
* [[Tryptophan]]
The amino acid tryptophan is a building block of the protein found in foods. It contributes to sleepiness. Carbohydrates make tryptophan more available to the brain, which is why carbohydrate-heavy meals containing tryptophan tend to cause drowsiness. Tryptophan is a precursor to the neurotransmitter serotonin, which is a precursor to the neurohormone melatonin (see below).

* [[Melatonin]]
Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone that regulates sleepiness. It is made in the brain where tryptophan is converted into serotonin and then into melatonin, which is released at night by the [[pineal gland]] to induce and maintain sleep. Melatonin supplementation may be used as a sleep aid, both as a [[hypnotic]] and as a [[chronobiotic]] (see [[phase response curve]], PRC).

*[[5-hydroxy-tryptophan|5-hydroxy-tryptophan (5-HTP)]]
5-HTP, the precursor to [[serotonin|serotonin (5-HT)]] can cause drowsiness when ingested.

* The "Post-Lunch Dip"
Many people have a temporary drop in alertness in early afternoon, commonly known as the ''post-lunch dip''. While a large meal, rich in carbohydrates, can make a person feel sleepy, the post-lunch dip is mostly an effect of the [[circadian rhythm|biological clock]]. People naturally feel most sleepy (have the greatest "drive for sleep") at two times of the day about 12 hours apart, for example at 2:00 AM and 2:00 PM. At those two times, the body clock "kicks in". At about 2 p.m. (14:00), it overrides the homeostatic build-up of sleep debt, allowing several more hours of wakefulness. At about 2 a.m. (02:00), with the daily sleep debt paid off, it "kicks in" again to ensure a few more hours of sleep.

* [[Alcoholic_beverage|Alcohol]]
Alcoholic beverages aid relaxation and commonly are used as a sleep aid. Alcohol tends, however, to disrupt sleep throughout the night and can prevent the deeper stages of sleep from occurring.

* [[Barbiturate]]s
Barbiturates when taken cause drowsiness and have actions similar to ethanol (alcohol).

=== Stimulation ===

*[[Caffeine]]
Caffeine is a stimulant that works by slowing the action of the hormones in the brain that cause sleepiness. Effective dosage is individual, in part dependent on prior usage. It can cause a rapid reduction in alertness as it wears off.
*[[Energy Drinks]]
The stimulating effects of energy drinks comes from natural stimulants such as caffeine, sugars, and essential amino acids, and eventually will create a rapid reduction in alertness similar to that of caffeine.
*[[Amphetamines]]
Amphetamines ([[amphetamine]], [[dextroamphetamine]], [[methamphetamine]], etc) are often used to treat [[narcolepsy]] and [[ADHD]] disorders, the most common effects are decreased appetite, stimulation and insomnia, and increased alertness.
*[[Cocaine]] and [[Crack Cocaine]]
Similar in action to the amphetamines.
*[[MDMA]]
Commonly known as ecstacy, users are kept awake similar to amphetamines with intense euphoria, includes other similar drugs like [[MDA]], [[MMDA]], or [[bk-MDMA]].
*[[Methylphenidate]]
Commonly known as Ritalin, similar in action to amphetamines and cocaine.

== Causes of difficulty in sleeping ==
There are a great many possible reasons for sleeping poorly. Following [[sleep hygiene|sleep hygienic]] principles may solve problems of physical or emotional discomfort.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.anxiety-and-depression-solutions.com/wellness_concerns/sleep/sleep_problem_causes.php |title= What Causes Sleep Difficulty? |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last=Little |first= Nan |date= 2007-01-01 |format= |work= |publisher= Insight Journal |quote= }}</ref> When pain, illness, drugs or stress are the culprit, the cause must be treated. [[Sleep disorder]]s, including the [[sleep apnea]]s, [[narcolepsy]], primary insomnia, [[Nocturnal myoclonus|periodic limb movement disorder]] (PLMD), [[restless leg syndrome]] (RLS) and the [[circadian rhythm sleep disorder]]s, are treatable.

Elderly people may to some degree lose the ability to consolidate sleep. They need the same amount per day as they've always needed, but may need to take some of their sleep as daytime naps.

== Anthropology of sleep ==
Recent research suggests that sleep patterns vary significantly across human [[cultures]].<ref>{{cite book | author = Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby | title=A comparative developmental ecology | chapter=6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep | chapterurl=http://webdrive.service.emory.edu/groups/research/lchb/PUBLICATIONS%20Worthman/PUBLICATIONS%20CMW%202002/Ecology%20of%20Human%20sleep.pdf | format=PDF | publisher=Emory University}}</ref><ref>[http://www.sciencenews.org/pages/sn_arc99/9_25_99/bob2.htm Slumber's Unexplored Landscape, Science News Online (9/25/99)<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> The most striking differences are between societies that have plentiful [[artificial light]] and ones that do not. Cultures without artificial light have more broken-up sleep patterns. This is called [[polyphasic sleep]] or [[segmented sleep]] and has led to expressions such as "first sleep," "watch," and "second sleep" which appear in literature from all over the world.

Some cultures have fragmented sleep patterns in which people sleep at all times of the day, and for shorter periods at night. For example, many [[Mediterranean]] and [[Latin American]] cultures have a ''[[siesta]]'', in which people sleep for a period in the [[afternoon]]. In many [[nomadic]] or [[hunter-gatherer]] societies people sleep off and on throughout the day or night depending on what is happening.{{Fact|date=January 2007}}

==Sleep in non-humans==
[[Image:Japanese Macaques sleeping.JPG|thumb|right|Sleeping [[Japanese Macaque]]s.]]

{{main|Sleep (non-human)}}

Horses and other herbivorous ungulates can sleep while standing, but must necessarily lie down for REM sleep (which causes muscular atony) for short periods - giraffes, for example, only need to lie down for REM sleep for a few minutes at a time. Bats sleep while hanging upside down. Some aquatic mammals and some birds can sleep with one half of the brain, while the other half is awake, so called [[unihemispheric slow-wave sleep]].<ref>{{cite journal | author = Mukhametova LM | coauthors = Supina AY, Polyakovaa IG | title = Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins | journal = Brain Research | volume = 134 | issue = 3 | pages = pp. 581–584 | publisher = |date=1977-10-14 | url = | doi = 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90835-6 | pmid = 902119 | accessdate = }}</ref> Birds and mammals have cycles of non-REM and REM sleep as described above for humans, though birds’ cycles are much shorter and they do not lose muscle tone (go limp) to the same extent that most mammals do.

Many animals sleep, but neurological sleep states are difficult to define in lower order animals. In these animals, sleep is defined using behavioral characteristics such as minimal movement, postures typical for the species and reduced responsiveness to external stimulation. It is quickly reversible, as opposed to hibernation or [[coma]], and sleep deprivation is followed by longer and/or deeper sleep.

Many species of mammals sleep for a large proportion of each 24-hour period when they are very young.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.madsci.org/posts/archives/2000-08/965504574.Zo.r.html |title= Re: Are there animals who don't sleep or that sleep very little? |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last= Faraco |first= Juliette |coauthors= |date= 2000-08-01 |format= |work= MadSci Network: Zoology |publisher= |pages= |quote= }}</ref> However, [[killer whale]]s and some [[dolphin]]s do not sleep during the first month of life.<ref>http://www.livescience.com/animals/050629_sleepless_sea.html</ref>

==Longest period without sleep==
Depending on how sleep is defined, there are several people who can claim the record for having gone the longest without sleep:

#[[Thai Ngoc]], born 1942, claimed in 2006 to have been awake for 33 years or 11,700 nights, according to [[Vietnam]]ese news organization Thanh Nien. It was said that Ngoc acquired the ability to go without sleep after a bout of fever in 1973,<ref name="Vu_2006">{{cite news | author=Vu Phuong Thao | translator=Thu Thuy | title=Vietnam man handles three decades without sleep | publisher=Thanh Nien |date=2006-02-14 | url=http://www.thanhniennews.com/features/?catid=10&newsid=12673}}</ref> but other reports indicate he stopped sleeping in 1976 with no known trigger.<ref name="Than_2007">{{cite news | author=Thanh Hai | title=My kingdom for a snooze | publisher=Vietnam Investment Review |date=2007-04-16 | url=http://www.vir.com.vn/Client/Timeout/index.asp?url=content.asp&doc=11739}}</ref> At the time of the Thanh Nien report, Ngoc suffered from no apparent ill effect (other than a minor decline in liver function), was mentally sound and could carry 100&nbsp;kg of pig feed down a 4&nbsp;km road,<ref name="Vu_2006" /> but another report indicates that he was healthy before the sleepless episode but that now he was not feeling well because of the lack of sleep.<ref name="Than_2007" />
#In January 2005, the [[RIA Novosti]] published an article about Fyodor Nesterchuk from the [[Ukraine|Ukrainian]] town of Kamen-Kashirsky who claimed to have not slept in more than 20 years. Local doctor Fyodor Koshel, chief of the [[Lutsk]] city health department, claimed to have examined him extensively and failed to make him sleep. Koshel also said however that Nesterchuck did not suffer any of the normally deleterious effects of sleep deprivation.<ref>http://www.xenophilia.com/str_biol05.htm</ref> People who claim not to sleep are usually shown to sleep when studied in sleep laboratories with EEG. Nesterchuck reports experiencing drowsiness at night, commenting that he attempts to sleep "in vain" when he notices his eyelids drooping. Many people experience [[microsleep]] episodes during sleep deprivation, in which they sleep for periods of seconds to fractions of a second and frequently don't remember these episodes. Because microsleep is frequently not remembered, microsleep or a related phenomenon may be responsible for lack of sleep and/or lack of memory of sleep in individuals like Nesterchuk and Thai Ngoc.
#[[Randy Gardner (record holder)|Randy Gardner]] holds the [[Guinness World Records|Guinness World Record]] for intentionally having gone the longest without sleep. In 1965, Gardner, then 18, stayed awake for 264 hours (about 11 days) for a high school science project.<ref name="SciAm">{{cite web|title=Biology: How long can humans stay awake? | publisher=Scientific American |date=2002-03-25 | url=http://www.sciam.com/askexpert_question.cfm?articleID=0000F879-8E01-1CD1-B4A8809EC588EEDF | accessdate = 2007-04-23}}</ref> He experienced significant deficits in concentration, motivation, perception and other higher mental processes during his sleep deprivation. However, he recovered normal cognitive functions after a few nights' sleep.
#On [[May 25]] [[2007]] the BBC reported that Tony Wright beat the Guinness World Record by staying awake for 11 days and nights.<ref name="bbc_2007">[http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/england/cornwall/6689999.stm BBC NEWS | England | Cornwall | Man claims new sleepless record<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> The Guinness Book of Records has, however, withdrawn its backing of a sleep deprivation class because of the associated health risks.
#A 3-year-old boy named Rhett Lamb of St. Petersburg Florida has a rare condition and has only slept for one to two hours per day in the past three years. He has a rare abnormality called an [[Arnold-Chiari malformation]] where brain tissue protrudes into the spinal canal. The skull puts pressure on the protruding part of the brain. It is not yet known if the brain malformation is directly related to his sleep deprivation.

==See also==
===Common sleeping positions, practices, and rituals===
* [[Co-sleeping]]
* [[Hypnosis]]
* [[Meditation]]
*[[Neutral spine]]
* [[Sleep hygiene]]
* [[Yoga Nidra]]

===Other===
* [[Microsleep]]
* [[Morvan's syndrome]]
* [[Alarm clock]]
* [[Dream world (plot device)]]
* [[Sudden infant death syndrome]]

==References==
{{Reflist|2}}

==Further reading==
* {{cite book | author=Bar-Yam, Yaneer | title = Dynamics of Complex Systems | chapter=Chapter 3 | chapterurl=http://necsi.org/publications/dcs/Bar-YamChap3.pdf | format=PDF | year = 2003}}
* {{cite journal | author = Foldvary-Schaefer N, Grigg-Damberger M | title = Sleep and epilepsy: what we know, don't know, and need to know. | journal = J Clin Neurophysiol | volume = 23 | issue = 1 | pages = 4–20 | year = 2006 | month=Feb | pmid = 16514348 | doi = 10.1097/01.wnp.0000206877.90232.cb}}
* {{cite journal | author = Gilmartin G, Thomas R | title = Mechanisms of arousal from sleep and their consequences. | journal = Curr Opin Pulm Med | volume = 10 | issue = 6 | pages = 468–74 | year = 2004 | month=Nov | pmid = 15510052 | doi = 10.1097/01.mcp.0000143690.94442.b3 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}} [Review]
* {{cite journal | author = Gottlieb D, Punjabi N, Newman A, Resnick H, Redline S, Baldwin C, Nieto F | title = Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance. | journal = Arch Intern Med | volume = 165 | issue = 8 | pages = 863–7 | year = 2005 | month=Apr 25 | pmid = 15851636 | doi = 10.1001/archinte.165.8.863 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Legramante J, Galante A | title = Sleep and hypertension: a challenge for the autonomic regulation of the cardiovascular system. | journal = Circulation | volume = 112 | issue = 6 | pages = 786–8 | year = 2005 | month=Aug 9 | pmid = 16087808 | doi= 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.555714}} [Editorial]
* Feinberg I. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed&cmd=Search&defaultField=Title+Word&term=J+Psychiatr+Res%5Bjour%5D+AND+10%5Bvolume%5D+AND+283%5Bpage%5D+AND+1974%5Bpdat%5D+AND+Feinberg+I%5Bauth%5D J Psychiatr Res. 1974;10:283–306.] [review]
* Tamar Shochat and Sonia Ancoli - [http://sleep.health.am/sleep/more/specific-clinical-patterns-in-aging/ Specific Clinical Patterns in Aging] - Sleep and Sleep Disorders [website]
* Zepelin H. Normal age related changes in sleep. In: Chase M, Weitzman ED, eds. Sleep Disorders: Basic and Clinical Research. New York: SP Medical; 1983:431–434.
* {{cite journal | author = Morrissey M, Duntley S, Anch A, Nonneman R | title = Active sleep and its role in the prevention of apoptosis in the developing brain. | journal = Med Hypotheses | volume = 62 | issue = 6 | pages = 876–9 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15142640 | doi = 10.1016/j.mehy.2004.01.014 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Marks G, Shaffery J, Oksenberg A, Speciale S, Roffwarg H | title = A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. | journal = Behav Brain Res | volume = 69 | issue = 1–2 | pages = 1–11 | year = 1995 | month = Jul-Aug | pmid = 7546299 | doi = 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-O <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Mirmiran M, Scholtens J, van de Poll N, Uylings H, van der Gugten J, Boer G | title = Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat. | journal = Brain Res | volume = 283 | issue = 2–3 | pages = 277–86 | year = 1983 | month = Apr | pmid = 6850353}}
* {{cite journal | author=Zhang, J. | title = [Memory process and the function of sleep] | journal=Journal of Theoretics | volume=6 | issue = 6 | year = 2004 | month = Dec | url=http://www.journaloftheoretics.com/Articles/6-6/Zhang.pdf | format=PDF}}

==External links==
{{wiktionary}}
* [http://www.aasmnet.org American Academy of Sleep Medicine]
* [http://www.sleepresearchsociety.org Sleep Research Society]
* [http://www.sleepfoundation.org National Sleep Foundation]
* [http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/about/ncsdr/index.htm National Center on Sleep Disorders Research]

{{SleepSeries2}}

[[Category:Sleep]]
[[Category:Unsolved problems in neuroscience]]


[[ar:نوم]]
[[ar:نوم]]
[[be:Сон]]
[[be:Сон]]
[[be-x-old:Сон]]
[[be-x-old:Сон]]
[[bg:Сън]]
[[bs:Spavanje]]
[[bs:Spavanje]]
[[bg:Сън]]
[[ca:Son]]
[[ca:Son]]
[[cs:Spánek]]
[[cs:Spánek]]
Linha 173: Linha 273:
[[da:Søvn]]
[[da:Søvn]]
[[de:Schlaf]]
[[de:Schlaf]]
[[et:Uni]]
[[el:Ύπνος]]
[[el:Ύπνος]]
[[en:Sleep]]
[[eo:Dormo]]
[[es:Sueño]]
[[es:Sueño]]
[[et:Uni]]
[[eo:Dormo]]
[[eu:Lo]]
[[eu:Lo]]
[[fa:خواب]]
[[fa:خواب]]
[[fi:Nukkuminen]]
[[fr:Sommeil]]
[[fr:Sommeil]]
[[gan:睏覺]]
[[gd:Cadal]]
[[gd:Cadal]]
[[gl:Sono]]
[[gl:Sono]]
[[he:שינה]]
[[gan:睏覺]]
[[ko:잠]]
[[hr:Spavanje]]
[[hr:Spavanje]]
[[hu:Alvás]]
[[id:Tidur]]
[[id:Tidur]]
[[is:Svefn]]
[[is:Svefn]]
[[it:Sonno]]
[[it:Sonno]]
[[ja:睡眠]]
[[he:שינה]]
[[ko:잠]]
[[lt:Miegas]]
[[lt:Miegas]]
[[hu:Alvás]]
[[ml:ഉറക്കം]]
[[ml:ഉറക്കം]]
[[mr:झोप]]
[[mr:झोप]]
[[ms:Tidur]]
[[ms:Tidur]]
[[new:न्ह्यः]]
[[nl:Slaap (rust)]]
[[nl:Slaap (rust)]]
[[new:न्ह्यः]]
[[nn:Søvn]]
[[ja:睡眠]]
[[no:Søvn]]
[[no:Søvn]]
[[nn:Søvn]]
[[uz:Uyqu]]
[[pl:Sen]]
[[pl:Sen]]
[[qu:Puñuy]]
[[pt:Sono]]
[[ro:Somn]]
[[ro:Somn]]
[[qu:Puñuy]]
[[ru:Сон]]
[[ru:Сон]]
[[sq:Gjumi]]
[[scn:Sonnu]]
[[scn:Sonnu]]
[[sh:Spavanje]]
[[simple:Sleep]]
[[simple:Sleep]]
[[sk:Spánok]]
[[sk:Spánok]]
[[sl:Uspavalo]]
[[sl:Uspavalo]]
[[sq:Gjumi]]
[[sh:Spavanje]]
[[su:Saré]]
[[su:Saré]]
[[fi:Nukkuminen]]
[[sv:Sömn]]
[[sv:Sömn]]
[[tg:Хоб]]
[[tg:Хоб]]
Linha 217: Linha 318:
[[uk:Сон (значення)]]
[[uk:Сон (значення)]]
[[ur:نیند]]
[[ur:نیند]]
[[uz:Uyqu]]
[[yi:שלאף]]
[[yi:שלאף]]
[[zh-yue:睏覺]]
[[zh:睡眠]]
[[zh:睡眠]]
[[zh-yue:睏覺]]

Revisão das 17h57min de 29 de julho de 2008

Predefinição:Otheruses

Sleepy men, Tehran, Iran

Sleep is a natural state of bodily rest observed throughout the animal kingdom. It is common to all mammals and birds, and is also seen in many reptiles, amphibians and fish. In humans, other mammals, and a substantial majority of other animals which have been studied — such as fish, birds, ants, and fruit-flies — regular sleep is essential for survival.[1] However, its purposes are only partly clear and are the subject of intense research.[2]

Physiology

In mammals and birds the measurement of eye movement during sleep is used to divide sleep into the two broad types of Rapid Eye Movement (REM) and Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM) sleep. Each type has a distinct set of associated physiological, neurological and psychological features.

Sleep proceeds in cycles of REM and the four stages of NREM, the order normally being:

stages 1 > 2 > 3 > 4 > 3 > 2 > REM.

In humans this cycle is on average 90 to 110 minutes,[3] with a greater amount of stages 3 and 4 early in the night and more REM later in the night. Each phase may have a distinct physiological function. Drugs such as sleeping pills and alcoholic beverages can suppress certain stages of sleep (see Sleep deprivation). This can result in a sleep that exhibits loss of consciousness but does not fulfill its physiological functions.

Allan Rechtschaffen and Anthony Kales originally outlined the criteria for staging sleep in 1968. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) updated the staging rules in 2007.

Stage 4 Sleep. EEG highlighted by red box.
REM Sleep. EEG highlighted by red box. Eye movements highlighted by red line.

Stages of sleep

Criteria for REM sleep include not only rapid eye movements but also a rapid low voltage EEG. In mammals, at least, low muscle tone is also seen. Most memorable dreaming occurs in this stage. NREM accounts for 75–80% of total sleep time in normal human adults. In NREM sleep, there is relatively little dreaming. Non-REM encompasses four stages; stages 1 and 2 are considered 'light sleep', and 3 and 4 'deep sleep' or slow-wave sleep, SWS. They are differentiated solely using EEG, unlike REM sleep which is characterized by rapid eye movements and relative absence of muscle tone. In non-REM sleep there are often limb movements, and parasomnias such as sleepwalking may occur. A cyclical alternating pattern may sometimes be observed during a stage.

NREM consists of four stages according to the 2007 AASM standards:

  • During Stage N1 the brain transitions from alpha waves (having a frequency of 8 to 13 Hz, common to people who are awake) to theta waves (frequency of 4 to 7 Hz). This stage is sometimes referred to as somnolence, or "drowsy sleep". Associated with the onset of sleep during N1 may be sudden twitches and hypnic jerks also known as positive myoclonus. Some people may also experience hypnagogic hallucinations during this stage, which can be more troublesome to the subject. During N1 the subject loses some muscle tone and conscious awareness of the external environment.
  • Stage N2, is characterized by "sleep spindles" (12 to 16 Hz) and "K-complexes." During this stage, muscular activity as measured by electromyography (EMG) lowers and conscious awareness of the external environment disappears. This stage occupies 45 to 55% of total sleep.
  • In Stage N3, the delta waves, also called delta rhythms (0.5 to 4 Hz) make up less than 50% of the total wave-patterns. This is considered part of deep or slow-wave sleep (SWS) and appears to function primarily as a transition into stage N4. This is the stage in which night terrors, bedwetting, sleepwalking and sleep-talking occur.
  • In Stage N4, delta-waves make up more than 50% of the wave-patterns. Stages N3 and N4 are the deepest forms of sleep; N4 is effectively a deeper version of N3, in which the deep-sleep characteristics, such as delta-waves, are more pronounced. As of new AASM guidelines, the determination between stage 3 and stage 4 sleep is inconsequential and both may be considered delta sleep or slow wave sleep. Therefore, in a recent ruling by the AASM, in order to make precision the scoring guidelines, stage four had been disbanded, and left is the stage of sleep N3 to describe the delta sleep attributed to it.[4]

Both REM sleep and NREM sleep stages 3 and 4 are homeostatically driven; that is, if a person or animal is selectively deprived of one of these, it rebounds once uninhibited sleep again is allowed. This suggests that both are essential to the functions of the sleep process.

Sleep timing

Sleep timing is controlled by the circadian clock, by homeostasis and, in humans, by willed behavior. The circadian clock, an inner time-keeping, temperature-fluctuating, enzyme-controlling device, works in tandem with adenosine, a neurotransmitter which inhibits many of the bodily processes that are associated with wakefulness. Adenosine is created over the course of the day; high levels of adenosine lead to sleepiness. In diurnal animals, sleepiness occurs as the circadian element causes the release of the hormone melatonin and a gradual decrease in core body temperature. The timing is affected by one's chronotype. It is the circadian rhythm which determines the ideal timing of a correctly structured and restorative sleep episode.[5]

Homeostatic sleep propensity, the need for sleep as a function of the amount of time elapsed since the last adequate sleep episode, is also important and must be balanced against the circadian element for satisfactory sleep. Along with corresponding messages from the circadian clock, this tells the body it needs to sleep.[6] Sleep offset, awakening, is primarily determined by circadian rhythm. A normal person who regularly awakens at an early hour will generally not be able to sleep much later than the person's normal waking time, even if moderately sleep deprived.

Optimal amount in humans

Adults

The optimal amount of sleep is not a meaningful concept unless the timing of that sleep is seen in relation to an individual's circadian rhythms. A person's major sleep episode is relatively inefficient and inadequate when it occurs at the "wrong" time of day. The timing is correct when the following two circadian markers occur after the middle of the sleep episode but before awakening:[7]

  • maximum concentration of the hormone melatonin, and
  • minimum core body temperature.

The National Sleep Foundation in the United States maintains that eight to nine hours of sleep for adult humans is optimal and that sufficient sleep benefits alertness, memory and problem solving, and overall health, as well as reducing the risk of accidents.[8] A widely publicized 2003 study[9] performed at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.

However, a University of California, San Diego psychiatry study of more than one million adults found that people who live the longest self-report sleeping for six to seven hours each night.[10] Another study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women showed similar results.[11] Other studies show that "sleeping more than 7 to 8 hours per day has been consistently associated with increased mortality", though this study suggests the cause is probably other factors such as depression and socio-economic status which would correlate statistically. [12] It has been suggested that the correlation between lower sleep hours and reduced morbidity only occurs with those who wake after less sleep naturally, rather than those who use an alarm.

A Koli Wada woman sleeping in Nirona village

Causal links are currently speculative: the available data may only reflect comorbid depression, socioeconomic status, or even alcohol use, for example.[13] These studies cannot be used to determine optimal sleep habits, only correlation — and empirically observed correlation is a necessary but not sufficient condition for causality. A need for nine or ten hours of sleep a day, or only five to six, may or may not have the same cause as the shortened life span. In other words, long or short sleep duration itself has not been shown to be a cause of early death.

Researchers from the University of Warwick and University College London have found that lack of sleep can more than double the risk of death from cardiovascular disease, but that too much sleep can also double the risk of death.[14][15] Professor Francesco Cappuccio said: “Short sleep has been shown to be a risk factor for weight gain, hypertension and Type 2 diabetes sometimes leading to mortality but in contrast to the short sleep-mortality association it appears that no potential mechanisms by which long sleep could be associated with increased mortality have yet been investigated. Some candidate causes for this include depression, low socioeconomic status and cancer-related fatigue. [...] In terms of prevention, our findings indicate that consistently sleeping around 7 hours per night is optimal for health and a sustained reduction may predispose to ill-health.”

Hours by age

A child sleeping

Children need a greater amount of sleep per day than adults to develop and function properly: up to 18 hours for newborn babies, with a declining rate as a child ages.[8][16] A newborn baby spends almost half of its sleep time in REM-sleep. By the age of five or so, only a bit over two hours are spent in REM.[17]

Age Average amount of sleep per day
Newborn up to 18 hours
1-12 months 14–18 hours
1-3 years 12-15 hours
3-5 years 11-13 hours
5-12 years 9-11 hours
Adolescents 9-10 hours
Adults, including elderly 7-8 (+) hours
Pregnant women 8 (+) hours

Sleep debt

Ver artigo principal: Sleep debt

Sleep debt is the effect of not getting quite enough rest and sleep; a large debt causes mental, emotional and physical fatigue. Scientists do not agree on how much sleep debt it is possible to accumulate, nor on whether the prevalence of sleep debt among adults has changed appreciably in the industrialized world in recent decades. It is likely that children are sleeping less than previously in Western societies.[18]

Functions

The multiple theories proposed to explain the function of sleep are reflective of the as yet incomplete understanding of the subject.

It is likely that sleep evolved to fulfill some primeval function, but has taken over multiple functions over time as organisms have evolved. An analogy would be that of the larynx, which performs multiple functions such as controlling the passage of food and air, phonation for communicating and social purposes, etc. These are all functions of the larynx but just one of them likely represents the original function. Some of the many proposed functions of sleep are as follows:

Restoration

Wound healing has been shown to be affected by sleep. A study conducted by Gumustekin et al.[19] in 2004 shows sleep deprivation hindering the healing of burns on rats.

It has also been shown that sleep deprivation affects the immune system and metabolism. In a study by Zager et al in 2007,[20] rats were deprived of sleep for 24 hours. When compared with a control group, the sleep-deprived rats' blood tests indicated a 20% decrease in white blood cell count, a significant change in the immune system.

A study by Bonnet and Arand[21] in 2003 indicates that sleep affects metabolism. Comparing normal human sleepers and sleepers with sleep state misperception insomnia, where patients complain of poor sleep but have normal sleep by electroencephalographic (EEG) criteria, the researchers found significantly greater metabolism values for the normal sleepers.

It has yet to be clearly proven that sleep duration affects somatic growth. One study by Jenni et al[22] in 2007 recorded growth, height and weight, as correlated to parent-reported time-in-bed in 305 children over a period of nine years (age 1-10). It was found that "the variation of sleep duration among children does not seem to have an effect on growth". It has been shown that sleep, more specifically slow-wave sleep (SWS), does affect growth hormone levels in adult men. During eight hours sleep, Van Cauter, Leproult, and Plat[23] found that the men with a high percentage of SWS (average 24%) also had high growth hormone secretion, while subjects with a low percentage of SWS (average 9%) had low growth hormone secretion.

There are multiple arguments supporting the restorative function of sleep. We feel rested after sleeping, and it is natural to assume that this is a basic purpose of sleep. Overall metabolic rate goes down during sleep and certain anabolic hormones such as growth hormones as mentioned above are secreted preferentially during sleep. Sleep among species is, in general, inversely related to the animal size and basal metabolic rate. Rats with a very high basal metabolic rate sleep for up to 14 hours a day whereas elephants and giraffes with lower BMRs sleep only 3-4 hours per day.

Energy conservation could as well have been accomplished by resting quiescent without shutting off the organism from the environment, potentially a dangerous situation. A sedentary non-sleeping animal is more likely to survive predators, while still preserving energy. Sleep therefore does something else other than conserving energy. Most interestingly, hibernating animals that wake up from hibernation go into rebound sleep because of lack of sleep during the hibernation period. They are definitely well rested and are conserving energy during hibernation, but need sleep for something else.[24] Rats kept awake indefinitely start dying after a few days.[25]

Anabolic/catabolic

Non-REM sleep may be an anabolic state marked by physiological processes of growth and rejuvenation of the organism's immune, nervous, muscular, and skeletal systems (but see above). Wakefulness may perhaps be viewed as a cyclical, temporary, hyperactive catabolic state during which the organism acquires nourishment and procreates.

Ontogenesis

According to the ontogenetic hypothesis of REM sleep, the activity occurring during neonatal REM sleep (or active sleep) seems to be particularly important to the developing organism (Marks et al., 1995). Studies investigating the effects of deprivation of active sleep have shown that deprivation early in life can result in behavioral problems, permanent sleep disruption, decreased brain mass (Mirmiran et al. 1983), and an abnormal amount of neuronal cell death (Morrissey, Duntley & Anch, 2004).

REM sleep appears to be important for development of the brain. REM sleep occupies majority of time of sleep of infants, which spend most of their time sleeping. Among different species, the more immature the baby is born, the more time it spends in REM sleep. Proponents also suggest that REM-induced muscle inhibition in the presence of brain activation exists to allow for brain development by activating the synapses yet without any motor consequences which may get the infant in trouble. Additionally, REM deprivation results in developmental abnormalities later in life.

However, this does not explain why older adults still need REM sleep, and why the fraction of time spent does not change significantly [carece de fontes?] as one ages. Aquatic mammal infants do not have REM sleep in infancy. REM sleep in those animals increases as they age. Obviously, REM sleep is not needed for development in these animals.

Memory processing

Scientists have shown numerous ways in which sleep is related to memory. In a study conducted by Turner, Drummond, Salamat, and Brown[26] working memory was shown to be affected by sleep deprivation. Working memory is important because it keeps information active for further processing and supports higher-level cognitive functions such as decision making, reasoning, and episodic memory. Turner et al. allowed 18 women and 22 men to sleep only 26 minutes per night over a 4-day period. Subjects were given initial cognitive tests while well rested and then tested again twice a day during the 4 days of sleep deprivation. On the final test the average working memory span of the sleep deprived group had dropped by 38% in comparison to the control group.

Memory also seems to be affected differently by certain stages of sleep such as REM and slow-wave sleep (SWS). In one study cited in Born, Rasch, and Gais[27] multiple groups of human subjects were used: wake control groups and sleep test groups. Sleep and wake groups were taught a task and then tested on it both on early and late nights, with the order of nights balanced across participants. When the subjects' brains were scanned during sleep, hypnograms revealed that SWS was the dominant sleep stage during the early night representing around 23% on average for sleep stage activity. The early night test group performed 16% better on the declarative memory test than the control group. During late night sleep, REM became the most active sleep stage at about 24%, and the late night test group performed 25% better on the procedural memory test than the control group. This indicates that procedural memory benefits from late REM-rich sleep whereas declarative memory benefits from early SWS-rich sleep.

Another study conducted by Datta[28] indirectly supports these results. The subjects chosen were 22 male rats. A box was constructed where a single rat could move freely from one end to the other. The bottom of the box was made of a steel grate. A light would shine in the box accompanied by a sound. After a 5 second delay an electrical shock would be applied. Once the shock commenced the rat could move to the other end of the box, ending the shock immediately. The rat could also use the 5-second delay to move to the other end of the box and avoid the shock entirely. The length of the shock never exceeded 5 seconds. This was repeated 30 times for half the rats. The other half, the control group, was placed in the same trial but the rats were shocked regardless of their reaction. After each of the training sessions the rat would be placed in a recording cage for 6 hours of polygraphic recordings. This process was repeated for 3 consecutive days. This study found that during the post-trial sleep recording session rats spent 25.47% more time in REM sleep after learning trials than after control trials. These trials support the results of the Born et al. study, indicating an obvious correlation between REM sleep and procedural knowledge.

Another interesting observation of the Datta study is that the learning group spent 180% more time in SWS than did the control group during the post-trial sleep-recording session. This phenomenon is supported by a study performed by Kudrimoti, Barnes, and McNaughton.[29] This study shows that after spatial exploration activity, patterns of hippocampal place cells are reactivated during SWS following the experiment. In a study by Kudrimoti et al. seven rats were run through a linear track using rewards on either end. The rats would then be placed in the track for 30 minutes to allow them to adjust (PRE), then they ran the track with reward based training for 30 minutes (RUN), and then they were allowed to rest for 30 minutes. During each of these three periods EEG data were collected for information on the rats’ sleep stages. Kudrimoti et al. computed the mean firing rates of hippocampal place cells during pre-behavior SWS (PRE) and three 10-minute intervals in post-behavior SWS (POST) by averaging across 22 track-running sessions from seven rats. The results showed that 10 minutes after the trial RUN session there was a 12% increase in the mean firing rate of hippocampal place cells from the PRE level, however after 20 minutes the mean firing rate returned rapidly toward the PRE level. The elevated firing of hippocampal place cells during SWS after spatial exploration could explain why there were elevated levels of SWS sleep in Datta’s study as it also dealt with a form of spatial exploration.

The different studies all suggest that there is a correlation between sleep and the many complex functions of memory.

Preservation

The "Preservation and Protection" theory holds that sleep serves an adaptive function. It protects the person during that portion of the 24-hour day in which being awake, and hence roaming around, would place the individual at greatest risk. Organisms do not require 24 hours to feed themselves and meet other necessities. From this perspective of adaptation, organisms are safer by staying out of harm's way where potentially they could be prey to other, stronger organisms. They sleep at times that maximize their safety, given their physical capacities and their habitats. (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Webb, 1982).

However, this theory fails to explain why the brain disengages from the external environment during normal sleep. Another argument against the theory is that sleep is not simply a passive consequence of removing the animal from the environment, but is a "drive": animals alter their behaviors in order to obtain sleep. Therefore, circadian regulation is more than sufficient to explain periods of activity and quiescence that are adaptive to an organism, but the more peculiar specializations of sleep probably serve different and unknown functions.

Moreover, the preservation theory does not explain why carnivores like lions, which are on top of the food chain, sleep the most. By the preservation logic, these top carnivores should not need any sleep at all. Preservation does not explain why aquatic mammals sleep while moving. Lethargy during these vulnerable hours would do the same, and will be more advantageous because the animal will be quiescent but still be able to respond to environmental challenges like predators etc. Sleep rebound that occurs after a sleepless night will be maladaptive, but still occurs for a reason. For example, a zebra falling asleep the day after it spent the sleeping time running from a lion is more and not less vulnerable to predation.

Dreaming

Ver artigo principal: Dream

Dreaming is the perception of sensory images during sleep, in a sequence which the sleeper/dreamer usually perceives more as an apparent participant than an observer. Dreaming is stimulated by the pons and mostly occurs during the REM phase of sleep.

People have proposed many hypotheses about the functions of dreaming. Sigmund Freud postulated that dreams are the symbolic expression of frustrated desires that had been relegated to the subconscious, and he used dream interpretation in the form of psychoanalysis to uncover these desires. Scientists have become skeptical about the Freudian interpretation, and place more emphasis on dreaming as a requirement for organization and consolidation of recent memory and experience. See Freud:The Interpretation of Dreams

Rosalind Cartwright stated that

One Function of dreams may be to restore our sense of competence.... it is also probable that in many times of stress, dreams have more work to do in resolving our problems and are thus more salient and memorable.[30]

Rosalind Cartwright, The Sunday Observer

J. Allan Hobson's and Robert McCarley's activation synthesis theory proposes that dreams are caused by the random firing of neurons in the cerebral cortex during the REM period. According to the theory, the forebrain then creates a story in an attempt to reconcile and make sense of the nonsensical sensory information presented to it, hence the odd nature of many dreams.[31]

Effect of food and drink on sleep

Drowsiness

The amino acid tryptophan is a building block of the protein found in foods. It contributes to sleepiness. Carbohydrates make tryptophan more available to the brain, which is why carbohydrate-heavy meals containing tryptophan tend to cause drowsiness. Tryptophan is a precursor to the neurotransmitter serotonin, which is a precursor to the neurohormone melatonin (see below).

Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone that regulates sleepiness. It is made in the brain where tryptophan is converted into serotonin and then into melatonin, which is released at night by the pineal gland to induce and maintain sleep. Melatonin supplementation may be used as a sleep aid, both as a hypnotic and as a chronobiotic (see phase response curve, PRC).

5-HTP, the precursor to serotonin (5-HT) can cause drowsiness when ingested.

  • The "Post-Lunch Dip"

Many people have a temporary drop in alertness in early afternoon, commonly known as the post-lunch dip. While a large meal, rich in carbohydrates, can make a person feel sleepy, the post-lunch dip is mostly an effect of the biological clock. People naturally feel most sleepy (have the greatest "drive for sleep") at two times of the day about 12 hours apart, for example at 2:00 AM and 2:00 PM. At those two times, the body clock "kicks in". At about 2 p.m. (14:00), it overrides the homeostatic build-up of sleep debt, allowing several more hours of wakefulness. At about 2 a.m. (02:00), with the daily sleep debt paid off, it "kicks in" again to ensure a few more hours of sleep.

Alcoholic beverages aid relaxation and commonly are used as a sleep aid. Alcohol tends, however, to disrupt sleep throughout the night and can prevent the deeper stages of sleep from occurring.

Barbiturates when taken cause drowsiness and have actions similar to ethanol (alcohol).

Stimulation

Caffeine is a stimulant that works by slowing the action of the hormones in the brain that cause sleepiness. Effective dosage is individual, in part dependent on prior usage. It can cause a rapid reduction in alertness as it wears off.

The stimulating effects of energy drinks comes from natural stimulants such as caffeine, sugars, and essential amino acids, and eventually will create a rapid reduction in alertness similar to that of caffeine.

Amphetamines (amphetamine, dextroamphetamine, methamphetamine, etc) are often used to treat narcolepsy and ADHD disorders, the most common effects are decreased appetite, stimulation and insomnia, and increased alertness.

Similar in action to the amphetamines.

Commonly known as ecstacy, users are kept awake similar to amphetamines with intense euphoria, includes other similar drugs like MDA, MMDA, or bk-MDMA.

Commonly known as Ritalin, similar in action to amphetamines and cocaine.

Causes of difficulty in sleeping

There are a great many possible reasons for sleeping poorly. Following sleep hygienic principles may solve problems of physical or emotional discomfort.[32] When pain, illness, drugs or stress are the culprit, the cause must be treated. Sleep disorders, including the sleep apneas, narcolepsy, primary insomnia, periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD), restless leg syndrome (RLS) and the circadian rhythm sleep disorders, are treatable.

Elderly people may to some degree lose the ability to consolidate sleep. They need the same amount per day as they've always needed, but may need to take some of their sleep as daytime naps.

Anthropology of sleep

Recent research suggests that sleep patterns vary significantly across human cultures.[33][34] The most striking differences are between societies that have plentiful artificial light and ones that do not. Cultures without artificial light have more broken-up sleep patterns. This is called polyphasic sleep or segmented sleep and has led to expressions such as "first sleep," "watch," and "second sleep" which appear in literature from all over the world.

Some cultures have fragmented sleep patterns in which people sleep at all times of the day, and for shorter periods at night. For example, many Mediterranean and Latin American cultures have a siesta, in which people sleep for a period in the afternoon. In many nomadic or hunter-gatherer societies people sleep off and on throughout the day or night depending on what is happening.[carece de fontes?]

Sleep in non-humans

Sleeping Japanese Macaques.
Ver artigo principal: Sleep (non-human)

Horses and other herbivorous ungulates can sleep while standing, but must necessarily lie down for REM sleep (which causes muscular atony) for short periods - giraffes, for example, only need to lie down for REM sleep for a few minutes at a time. Bats sleep while hanging upside down. Some aquatic mammals and some birds can sleep with one half of the brain, while the other half is awake, so called unihemispheric slow-wave sleep.[35] Birds and mammals have cycles of non-REM and REM sleep as described above for humans, though birds’ cycles are much shorter and they do not lose muscle tone (go limp) to the same extent that most mammals do.

Many animals sleep, but neurological sleep states are difficult to define in lower order animals. In these animals, sleep is defined using behavioral characteristics such as minimal movement, postures typical for the species and reduced responsiveness to external stimulation. It is quickly reversible, as opposed to hibernation or coma, and sleep deprivation is followed by longer and/or deeper sleep.

Many species of mammals sleep for a large proportion of each 24-hour period when they are very young.[36] However, killer whales and some dolphins do not sleep during the first month of life.[37]

Longest period without sleep

Depending on how sleep is defined, there are several people who can claim the record for having gone the longest without sleep:

  1. Thai Ngoc, born 1942, claimed in 2006 to have been awake for 33 years or 11,700 nights, according to Vietnamese news organization Thanh Nien. It was said that Ngoc acquired the ability to go without sleep after a bout of fever in 1973,[38] but other reports indicate he stopped sleeping in 1976 with no known trigger.[39] At the time of the Thanh Nien report, Ngoc suffered from no apparent ill effect (other than a minor decline in liver function), was mentally sound and could carry 100 kg of pig feed down a 4 km road,[38] but another report indicates that he was healthy before the sleepless episode but that now he was not feeling well because of the lack of sleep.[39]
  2. In January 2005, the RIA Novosti published an article about Fyodor Nesterchuk from the Ukrainian town of Kamen-Kashirsky who claimed to have not slept in more than 20 years. Local doctor Fyodor Koshel, chief of the Lutsk city health department, claimed to have examined him extensively and failed to make him sleep. Koshel also said however that Nesterchuck did not suffer any of the normally deleterious effects of sleep deprivation.[40] People who claim not to sleep are usually shown to sleep when studied in sleep laboratories with EEG. Nesterchuck reports experiencing drowsiness at night, commenting that he attempts to sleep "in vain" when he notices his eyelids drooping. Many people experience microsleep episodes during sleep deprivation, in which they sleep for periods of seconds to fractions of a second and frequently don't remember these episodes. Because microsleep is frequently not remembered, microsleep or a related phenomenon may be responsible for lack of sleep and/or lack of memory of sleep in individuals like Nesterchuk and Thai Ngoc.
  3. Randy Gardner holds the Guinness World Record for intentionally having gone the longest without sleep. In 1965, Gardner, then 18, stayed awake for 264 hours (about 11 days) for a high school science project.[41] He experienced significant deficits in concentration, motivation, perception and other higher mental processes during his sleep deprivation. However, he recovered normal cognitive functions after a few nights' sleep.
  4. On May 25 2007 the BBC reported that Tony Wright beat the Guinness World Record by staying awake for 11 days and nights.[42] The Guinness Book of Records has, however, withdrawn its backing of a sleep deprivation class because of the associated health risks.
  5. A 3-year-old boy named Rhett Lamb of St. Petersburg Florida has a rare condition and has only slept for one to two hours per day in the past three years. He has a rare abnormality called an Arnold-Chiari malformation where brain tissue protrudes into the spinal canal. The skull puts pressure on the protruding part of the brain. It is not yet known if the brain malformation is directly related to his sleep deprivation.

See also

Common sleeping positions, practices, and rituals

Other

References

  1. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council. [S.l.]: The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. pg 121. ISBN 978-0-309-08903-6. Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983). 
  2. Bingham, Roger; Terrence Sejnowski, Jerry Siegel, Mark Eric Dyken, Charles Czeisler, Paul Shaw, Ralph Greenspan, Satchin Panda, Philip Low, Robert Stickgold, Sara Mednick, Allan Pack, Luis de Lecea, David Dinges, Dan Kripke, Giulio Tononi (2007). «Waking Up To Sleep» (Several conference videos). The Science Network. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  3. Swierzewski, Stanley J., M.D. (01 December 2000, reviewed 04 December 2007). «Sleep Stages. Overview, Waking, Non-REM, REM, Sleep Cycle, Factors, Age». Sleep Channel, Healthcommunities.com. Consultado em 10 de fevereiro de 2008  Verifique data em: |data= (ajuda)
  4. Psychology World (1998). «Stages of Sleep» (PDF). Consultado em 15 de junho de 2008 
  5. Wyatt, James K.; Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan (1999). «Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day». Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. Fulltext. PMID 10516257. Consultado em 25 de novembro de 2007 
  6. de Benedictis, Tina, Ph.D.; Heather Larson, Gina Kemp, M.A., Suzanne Barston, Robert Segal, M.A. (2007). «Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages». Helpguide.org. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  7. Wyatt, James K.; Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan (1999). «Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day». Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. PMID 10516257. Consultado em 25 de novembro de 2007. ... significant homeostatic and circadian modulation of sleep structure, with the highest sleep efficiency occurring in sleep episodes bracketing the melatonin maximum and core body temperature minimum 
  8. a b «"Let Sleep Work for You" provided by the National Sleep Foundation» 
  9. Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003 Mar 15;26(2):117–26.
  10. Rhonda Rowland (15 de fevereiro de 2002). «Experts challenge study linking sleep, life span». Consultado em 22 de abril de 2007 
  11. Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR et al (2004) A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women Sleep 1;27(3):440-4
  12. Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ et al (2006) Correlates of long sleep duration Sleep 29(7):881-889
  13. Irwin MR, Ziegler M (2005) Sleep deprivation potentiates activation of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses in abstinent alcoholics Hypertension 45(2):252-7
  14. «"Researchers say lack of sleep doubles risk of death… but so can too much sleep"» 
  15. Jane E. Ferrie, Martin J. Shipley, Francesco P. Cappuccio, Eric Brunner, Michelle A. Miller, Meena Kumari, and Michael G. Marmot. “A prospective study of change in sleep duration; associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort”. SLEEP Journal.
  16. de Benedictis, Tina, Ph.D.; Heather Larson, Gina Kemp, M.A., Suzanne Barston, Robert Segal, M.A. (2007). «Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages». Helpguide.org. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  17. Siegel, Jerome M. (1999). «Sleep». Encarta Encyclopedia. Microsoft. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  18. Iglowstein, Ivo; Oskar G. Jenni, MD, Luciano Molinari, PhD and Remo H. Largo, MD (2003). «Sleep Duration From Infancy to Adolescence: Reference Values and Generational Trends». Pediatrics. 111 (2): pp. 302–307. PMID 12563055. doi:10.1542/peds.111.2.302. Thus, the shift in the evening bedtime across cohorts accounted for the substantial decrease in sleep duration in younger children between the 1970s and the 1990s. ... [A] more liberal parental attitude toward evening bedtime in the past decades is most likely responsible for the bedtime shift and for the decline of sleep duration... 
  19. Gumustekin, K., Seven, B., Karabulut, N., Aktas, O., Gursan, N., Aslan, S., Keles, M., Varoglu, E., & Dane S. (2004). Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats [Electronic version]. Neuroscience, 114, 1433-1442.
  20. Zager, A., Andersen, M. L., Ruiz, F. S., Antunes, I. B., & Tufik, S. (2007). Effects of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats [Electronic version]. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 293, R504-R509.
  21. Bonnet, M. H. & Arand, D. L. (2003). Insomnia, metabolic rate and sleep restoration [Electronic version]. Journal of Internal Medicine, 254, 23-31.
  22. Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., Caflisch, J. A., & Largo, R. H. (2007). Sleep duration from ages 1 to 10 years: Variability and stability in comparison with growth [Electronic version]. Pediatrics, 120, e769-e776.
  23. Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., & Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow-wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men [Electronic version]. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 861-868.
  24. Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM (1991). «Warming up for sleep? Ground squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation». Neurosci. Lett. 128 (2). 581 páginas. PMID 1945046. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y 
  25. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council. [S.l.]: The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. pg 121. ISBN 978-0-309-08903-6. Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983). 
  26. Turner, T. H., Drummond, S. P. A., Salamat, J. S., & Brown, G. G. (2007). Effects of 42 hr sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory [Electronic version]. Neuropsychology, 21, 787-795.
  27. Born, J., Rasch, J., & Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember [Electronic version]. Neuroscientist, 12, 410.
  28. Datta, S. (2000). Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity [Electronic version]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8607-8613.
  29. Kudrimoti, H. S., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: Effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics [Electronic version]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 4090-4101.
  30. Nanayakkara, Anushka. «Dream your dreams away». The Sunday Observer. Consultado em 9 de julho de 2008 
  31. Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348.
  32. Little, Nan (1 de janeiro de 2007). «What Causes Sleep Difficulty?». Insight Journal. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  33. Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby. «6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep» (PDF). A comparative developmental ecology (PDF). [S.l.]: Emory University 
  34. Slumber's Unexplored Landscape, Science News Online (9/25/99)
  35. Mukhametova LM; Supina AY, Polyakovaa IG (14 de outubro de 1977). «Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins». Brain Research. 134 (3): pp. 581–584. PMID 902119. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90835-6 
  36. Faraco, Juliette (1 de agosto de 2000). «Re: Are there animals who don't sleep or that sleep very little?». MadSci Network: Zoology. Consultado em 25 de janeiro de 2008 
  37. http://www.livescience.com/animals/050629_sleepless_sea.html
  38. a b Vu Phuong Thao (14 de fevereiro de 2006). «Vietnam man handles three decades without sleep». Traduzido por Thu Thuy. Thanh Nien 
  39. a b Thanh Hai (16 de abril de 2007). «My kingdom for a snooze». Vietnam Investment Review 
  40. http://www.xenophilia.com/str_biol05.htm
  41. «Biology: How long can humans stay awake?». Scientific American. 25 de março de 2002. Consultado em 23 de abril de 2007 
  42. BBC NEWS | England | Cornwall | Man claims new sleepless record

Further reading

  • Bar-Yam, Yaneer (2003). «Chapter 3» (PDF). Dynamics of Complex Systems (PDF). [S.l.: s.n.] 
  • Foldvary-Schaefer N, Grigg-Damberger M (2006). «Sleep and epilepsy: what we know, don't know, and need to know.». J Clin Neurophysiol. 23 (1): 4–20. PMID 16514348. doi:10.1097/01.wnp.0000206877.90232.cb 
  • Gilmartin G, Thomas R (2004). «Mechanisms of arousal from sleep and their consequences.». Curr Opin Pulm Med. 10 (6): 468–74. PMID 15510052. doi:10.1097/01.mcp.0000143690.94442.b3  [Review]
  • Gottlieb D, Punjabi N, Newman A, Resnick H, Redline S, Baldwin C, Nieto F (2005). «Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance.». Arch Intern Med. 165 (8): 863–7. PMID 15851636. doi:10.1001/archinte.165.8.863 
  • Legramante J, Galante A (2005). «Sleep and hypertension: a challenge for the autonomic regulation of the cardiovascular system.». Circulation. 112 (6): 786–8. PMID 16087808. doi:10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.555714  [Editorial]
  • Feinberg I. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age J Psychiatr Res. 1974;10:283–306. [review]
  • Tamar Shochat and Sonia Ancoli - Specific Clinical Patterns in Aging - Sleep and Sleep Disorders [website]
  • Zepelin H. Normal age related changes in sleep. In: Chase M, Weitzman ED, eds. Sleep Disorders: Basic and Clinical Research. New York: SP Medical; 1983:431–434.
  • Morrissey M, Duntley S, Anch A, Nonneman R (2004). «Active sleep and its role in the prevention of apoptosis in the developing brain.». Med Hypotheses. 62 (6): 876–9. PMID 15142640. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2004.01.014 
  • Marks G, Shaffery J, Oksenberg A, Speciale S, Roffwarg H (1995). «A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation.». Behav Brain Res. 69 (1–2): 1–11. PMID 7546299. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-O 
  • Mirmiran M, Scholtens J, van de Poll N, Uylings H, van der Gugten J, Boer G (1983). «Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat.». Brain Res. 283 (2–3): 277–86. PMID 6850353 
  • Zhang, J. (2004). «[Memory process and the function of sleep]» (PDF). Journal of Theoretics. 6 (6) 

External links

Wikcionário
Wikcionário
O Wikcionário tem o verbete sono.

Predefinição:SleepSeries2 pt:Sono